Big Chemical Encyclopedia

Chemical substances, components, reactions, process design ...

Articles Figures Tables About

Sweetness perception

One reason for the seemingly slow progress of understanding is the interdisciplinary nature of sweetness research. The conclusions that can be drawn, from, for example, physiological and psychophysical experimentation, must be related to what is known of the structural chemistry of the stimulus and how it may interact at the molecular level. All too often, it is not appreciated that one particular line of experimentation cannot be viewed in isolation, but must relate to other disciplines. Only by fully understanding all of the associated events leading to sweetness perception shall we understand the mechanism of sweetness perception itself. [Pg.201]

It has been proposed " that the mechanism(s) of action of gymnemic acids and ziziphins is a biphasic, model-membrane penetration-process. The model suggested that the modifier molecules interact first with the receptor-cell plasma-membrane surface. It was postulated that this initial interaction involves a selective effect on taste perception, including the transduction and quality specification of the sweet stimuli, and selective depression of sweetness perception. Following the initial interaction, the modifier molecules interact with the membrane-lipid interior to produce a general disruption of membrane function and a nonselective effect on taste... [Pg.337]

Much of our present day knowledge of sweetness intensity, both at the threshold level, where taste begins, and above the threshold level, derives from the application of psychophysical techniques. It is now evident that the psychophysical procedure used measure separate aspects of sweetness perception. Hedonic responses cannot be predicted from intensity of discrimination data, and vice versa. The taste-panel evaluation of sweetness is of fundamental importance in the development of worthwhile structure-taste relationships. Therefore, it is vital that the appropriate psychophysical method and experimental procedure be adopted for a particular objective of investigation. Otherwise, false conclusions, or improper inferences, or both, result. This situation results from the failure to recognize that individual tests measure separate parameters of sensory behavior. It is not uncommon that the advocates of a specific method or procedure seldom... [Pg.349]

Taste characteristics in general determine applicability of intense sweeteners. A time-intensity profile of sweetness perception similar to sucrose is desirable, and a delay in sweetness onset or a lingering sweetness are generally perceived as less pleasant. Side-tastes like bitter, liquorice or metallic taste are disadvantages which limit the applicability of some sweeteners. [Pg.230]

Sweet Taste. The mechanism of sweetness perception has been extensively studied because of its commercial importance. Many substances that vary in chemical structure have been discovered which are similar to the taste of sucrose. Commercial sweeteners include sucralose, acesulfame-K, saccharin, aspartame, cyclamate (Canada) and the protein thaumatin 4), Each sweetener is unique in its perceived sensation because of the time to the onset of sweetness and to maximum sweetness, ability to mask other sensations, persistence, aftertaste and intensity relative to sucrose [TABLE IT. For example, the saccharides, sorbitol and... [Pg.11]

Bitterness mechanism, 28 receptor model, 29,31/,34,35/ receptor structure, 161,162/ similarity to sweetness perception, 31/ Bread, white, use of enzyme-active soya flour, 192-198... [Pg.343]

Brazzein is a sweet protein that was isolated from the fruit of the West African climbing plant Oubli (Pentadiplandra brazzeana Baillon). Along with pentadin, which was discovered in 1989, brazzein is the second sweet protein that was discovered in this fruit. Like other natural sweet proteins such as monellin and thaumatin, it is highly sweet. On a weight basis, brazzein is 500 times sweeter than sucrose when compared to 10% sucrose solution and 2000 times sweeter when compared to 2% sucrose solution. Its sweet perception is more similar to that of sucrose than that of thaumatin, and it presents a clean sweet taste with a lingering aftertaste. Brazzein is stable over a broad pH range from 2.5 to 8 and is heat stable at 80 °C for 4h.84... [Pg.640]

Sweeteners are natural or synthetic compounds which imprint a sweet sensation and possess no or negligible nutritional value ( nonnutritive sweeteners ) in relation to the extent of sweetness. There is considerable interest in new sweeteners. The rise in obesity in industrialized countries has established a trend for calorie-reduced nutrition. Also, there is an increased discussion about the safety of saccharin and cyclamate, the two sweeteners which were predominant for a long time. The search for new sweeteners is complicated by the fact that the relationship between chemical structure and sweetness perception is not yet satisfactorily resolved. In addition, the safety of suitable compounds has to be certain. Some other criteria must also be met, for example, the compound must be adequately soluble and stable over a wide pH and temperature range, have a clean sweet taste without side or post-flavor effects, and provide a sweetening effect as cost-effectively as does sucrose. At present, some new sweeteners are on the market (e. g., acesulfame and aspartame). The application of a number of other compounds will be discussed here. [Pg.432]

CM Christensen. Effects of solution viscosity on perceived saltiness and sweetness. Percept Psychophys 28 347-353, 1980. [Pg.186]

Sensory perception is both quaUtative and quantitative. The taste of sucrose and the smell of linalool are two different kinds of sensory perceptions and each of these sensations can have different intensities. Sweet, bitter, salty, fmity, floral, etc, are different flavor quaUties produced by different chemical compounds the intensity of a particular sensory quaUty is deterrnined by the amount of the stimulus present. The saltiness of a sodium chloride solution becomes more intense if more of the salt is added, but its quaUty does not change. However, if hydrochloric acid is substituted for sodium chloride, the flavor quahty is sour not salty. For this reason, quaUty is substitutive, and quantity, intensity, or magnitude is additive (13). The sensory properties of food are generally compHcated, consisting of many different flavor quaUties at different intensities. The first task of sensory analysis is to identify the component quahties and then to determine their various intensities. [Pg.1]

A persistent idea is that there is a very small number of flavor quaUties or characteristics, called primaries, each detected by a different kind of receptor site in the sensory organ. It is thought that each of these primary sites can be excited independently but that some chemicals can react with more than one site producing the perception of several flavor quaUties simultaneously (12). Sweet, sour, salty, bitter, and umami quaUties are generally accepted as five of the primaries for taste sucrose, hydrochloric acid, sodium chloride, quinine, and glutamate, respectively, are compounds that have these primary tastes. Sucrose is only sweet, quinine is only bitter, etc saccharin, however, is slightly bitter as well as sweet and its Stevens law exponent is 0.8, between that for purely sweet (1.5) and purely bitter (0.6) compounds (34). There is evidence that all compounds with the same primary taste characteristic have the same psychophysical exponent even though they may have different threshold values (24). The flavor of a complex food can be described as a combination of a smaller number of flavor primaries, each with an associated intensity. A flavor may be described as a vector in which the primaries make up the coordinates of the flavor space. [Pg.3]

Odor data for the various amyl alcohols is limited. The lowest perceptible limit for 1-pentanol and / fZ-amyl alcohol are 10 and 0.04 ppm, respectively (135). tert-Axa[. alcohol has a threshold value of 2.3 ppm (and a 100% recognition level of 0.23 ppm) 3-methyl-1-butanol has an odor threshold of 7.0 ppm. The odor of 1-pentanol has been described as sweet and pleasant whereas that of 3-methyl-2-butanol is sour (135). [Pg.375]

Also notable is the unique sweetness response profile of fmctose compared to other sweeteners (3,4). In comparison with dextrose and sucrose, the sweetness of fmctose is more quickly perceived on the tongue, reaches its iatensity peak earlier, and dissipates more rapidly. Thus, the sweetness of fmctose enhances many food flavor systems, eg, fmits, chocolate, and spices such as cinnamon, cloves, and salt. By virtue of its early perception and rapid diminution, fmctose does not have the flavor-maskiag property of other common sugars. [Pg.44]

Taste and Flavor. The taste effect is generally sweet, but depends strongly on the base of preparation. Eor tasting purposes, vanillin is often evaluated in ice-cold milk with about 12% sugar. A concentration of 50 ppm in this medium is clearly perceptible. Vanilla is undoubtedly one of the most popular flavors its consumption in the form of either vanilla extracts or vanillin is almost universal. [Pg.398]

Different polysaccharides change the perception of flavour, thus xanthan is superior to gum guar in the perception of sweetness. Mixtures of xanthan and locust bean gum have improved flavour release and texture when used in pies and pat s compared to starch. Many foods are emulsions, examples being soups, sauces and spreads. Exopolysaccharides are used to stabilise these emulsions and prevent the phases from... [Pg.225]

Food colorants play an important role in quality perception. Color is often the first notable characteristic of a food and it influences the expectations of consumers buying the product and also influences food handlers who make quality-related decisions, for example, during visual inspections." More specifically, color predetermines our expectations and perceptions of flavor and taste. " Color is interrelated with flavor intensity (detection threshold), with sweetness and salinity sensations, and also with our susceptibilities to and preferences for products. For example, consumers perceived a strongly red-colored strawberry-flavored drink to be sweeter than a less colored version, and yellow was associated with lemon and pink with grapefruit, but by reversing the colors, flavor perception changed." If food color is not appealing, consumers will not enjoy the flavor and texture of the food. ... [Pg.553]

Multiple senses, including taste, contribute to our total perception of food. Our perception of the flavor of food is a complex experience based upon multiple senses taste per se, which includes sweet, sour, salty and bitter olfaction, which includes aromas touch, also termed mouth feel , that is, texture and fat content and thermoreception and nociception caused by pungent spices and irritants. Taste proper is commonly divided into four categories of primary stimuli sweet, sour, salty and bitter. One other primary taste quality, termed umami (the taste of L-glutamate), is still somewhat controversial. Mixtures of these primaries can mimic the tastes of more complex foods. [Pg.825]

Reviews of taste sensations normally concentrate on four basic tastes - sweet, salty, sour and bitter (7,2) however, other oral sensations can contribute important information to the perceived flavor (3), Examples of stimulants evoking these very different sensory sensations are shown in TABLE I. Studies on the mechaiusms of perception are usually restricted to sensation-specific stimuli however, food flavors represent an interaction among the various sensations. This chapter describes recent... [Pg.10]

Results from sensory evaluation of mixed solution are seen in Table IV. The data list the theoretical response for both the independent and competitive receptor hypothesis as well as the actual sensory score. The actual sensory scores were found to agree fairly well with the competitive model. The minor dissimilarity between the actual and theoretical is due to the inability of individual to taste bitterness in solutions that are extremely sweet, i.e., there is some masking of overall sensory perception which is concentration dependent. The data, therefore, clearly indicate that sweetness and bitterness act in a competitive manner and should be considered to compete for the binding sites at the same receptor. [Pg.33]

Members of the Gs subfamily are activated by hormone receptors, by odor receptors and by taste receptors. Gg-proteins mediate, e.g., signal transmission by adrenaline receptors of type P and by glucagon receptors. During perception of taste, the taste receptors are activated, which then pass the signal on via the olfactory G-protein Gou. Perception of sweet taste is also mediated via a Gs-protein. Transmission of the... [Pg.193]


See other pages where Sweetness perception is mentioned: [Pg.31]    [Pg.467]    [Pg.35]    [Pg.643]    [Pg.415]    [Pg.185]    [Pg.683]    [Pg.26]    [Pg.240]    [Pg.240]    [Pg.265]    [Pg.31]    [Pg.467]    [Pg.35]    [Pg.643]    [Pg.415]    [Pg.185]    [Pg.683]    [Pg.26]    [Pg.240]    [Pg.240]    [Pg.265]    [Pg.1]    [Pg.2]    [Pg.10]    [Pg.292]    [Pg.11]    [Pg.200]    [Pg.248]    [Pg.340]    [Pg.242]    [Pg.196]    [Pg.502]    [Pg.5]    [Pg.10]    [Pg.10]    [Pg.28]    [Pg.34]    [Pg.34]    [Pg.105]    [Pg.135]   
See also in sourсe #XX -- [ Pg.201 ]




SEARCH



Perception

Sweetness perception, substances that

© 2024 chempedia.info