Big Chemical Encyclopedia

Chemical substances, components, reactions, process design ...

Articles Figures Tables About

Taste human

The other instinctively pleasurable taste is sweetness, which permits detection of carbohydrates, and hence energy sources. While it is only sugars (section 4.2.1) that have a sweet taste, human beings (and a few other animals) secrete the enzyme amylase in saliva (section 4.2.21) amylase catalyses the hydrolysis of starch, which is the major dietary carbohydrate, to sweet-tasting sugars while the food is being chewed. [Pg.10]

Ingredients used in pet foods are usually high in nutritional quaHty but generally not desirable as human foods primarily because they do not conform to human taste or processing expectations. By-products such as rendered proteins and fat converted into pet foods may have a derivation unappealing to humans, yet after processing may actually be more free of microorganisms and toxins than foods consumed by humans. [Pg.150]

The aroma of fmit, the taste of candy, and the texture of bread are examples of flavor perception. In each case, physical and chemical stmctures ia these foods stimulate receptors ia the nose and mouth. Impulses from these receptors are then processed iato perceptions of flavor by the brain. Attention, emotion, memory, cognition, and other brain functions combine with these perceptions to cause behavior, eg, a sense of pleasure, a memory, an idea, a fantasy, a purchase. These are psychological processes and as such have all the complexities of the human mind. Flavor characterization attempts to define what causes flavor and to determine if human response to flavor can be predicted. The ways ia which simple flavor active substances, flavorants, produce perceptions are described both ia terms of the physiology, ie, transduction, and psychophysics, ie, dose-response relationships, of flavor (1,2). Progress has been made ia understanding how perceptions of simple flavorants are processed iato hedonic behavior, ie, degree of liking, or concept formation, eg, crispy or umami (savory) (3,4). However, it is unclear how complex mixtures of flavorants are perceived or what behavior they cause. Flavor characterization involves the chemical measurement of iadividual flavorants and the use of sensory tests to determine their impact on behavior. [Pg.1]

Human perception creates difficulty ia the characterization of flavor people often, if not always, perceive flavors differently due to both psychological and physiological factors. For example, certain aryl thiocarbamates, eg, phenylthiocarbamide, taste exceedingly bitter to some people and are almost tasteless to others (5). This difference is genetically determined, and the frequency of its occurrence differs from one population to another 40% of U.S. Caucasians are nontasters, whereas only 3% of the Korean population caimot perceive the strong bitter taste of the aryl thiocarbamates (6). Similar differences were found ia the sense of smell for compounds such as menthol, carvone, and ethyl butyrate (7). [Pg.1]

Whatever the physiology of odor perception may be, the sense of smell is keener than that of taste (22). If flavors are classed into odors and tastes as is common practice in science, it can be calculated that there are probably more than 10 possible sensations of odor and only a few, perhaps five, sensations of taste (13,21,35—37). Just as a hereditary or genetic factor may cause taste variations between individuals toward phenylthiourea, a similar factor may be in operation with odor. The odor of the steroid androsterone, found in many foods and human sweat, may eflcit different responses from different individuals. Some are very sensitive to it and find it unpleasant. To others, who are less sensitive to it, it has a musk or sandalwood-like smell. Approximately 50% of the adults tested cannot detect any odor even at extremely high concentrations. It is befleved that this abiUty is genetically determined (38). [Pg.11]

Toxicity. Sodium fluoroacetate is one of the most effective all-purpose rodenticides known (18). It is highly toxic to all species of rats tested and can be used either in water solution or in bait preparations. Its absence of objectionable taste and odor and its delayed effects lead to its excellent acceptance by rodents. It is nonvolatile, chemically stable, and not toxic or irritating to the unbroken skin of workers. Rats do not appear to develop any significant tolerance to this compound from nonlethal doses. However, it is extremely dangerous to humans, to common household pets, and to farm animals, and should only be used by experienced personnel. The rodent carcasses should be collected and destroyed since they remain poisonous for a long period of time to any animal that eats them. [Pg.307]

Perfumes and Cosmetics. Vanillin, a crystal, is the main constituent of the vanilla bean. Its importance can be illustrated by the fact that human preferences in fragrances and in flavors, as determined by various studies, comprise three main smells or tastes rose, vanilla, and strawberry. [Pg.400]

The design of these devices is based on biological principles of organization of human olfactory and tasting abilities. [Pg.19]

Substances which have a deleterious effect on the taste and/or smell of the products for human consumption derived from the aquatic environment Toxic or persistent organic compounds of silicon Inorganic compounds of phosphorus and elemental phosphorus Non-persistent mineral oils and hydrocarbons of petroleum origin Cyanides, fluorides... [Pg.517]

Pollution The impairment (reduction) of water quality by agriculture, domestic or industrial wastes (including thermal and radioactive wastes) to such a degree as to hinder any beneficial use of the water or render it offensive to the senses of sight, taste, or smell or when sufficient amounts of waste creates or poses a potential threat to human health or the environment. [Pg.622]

Some proteins display rather exotic functions that do not quite fit the previous classifications. Monellin, a protein found in an African plant, has a very sweet taste and is being considered as an artificial sweetener for human consumption. Resilin, a protein having exceptional elastic properties, is found in... [Pg.125]

The metal lost from the inside of pumps, reaction vessels, pipework, etc. usually contaminates the product. The implications of this depend upon the product. Ppb levels of iron can discolor white plastics, though at this level the effect is purely cosmetic. Ppm levels of iron and other metals affect the taste of beer. Products sold to compositional requirements (such as reagent-grade acids) can be spoiled by metal pick-up. Pharmaceutical products for human use are often white tablets or powders and are easily discolored by slight contamination by corrosion products. [Pg.897]

What are they like to eat Humans are particular about the organoleptic properties of their food. Microbial cells may have little taste or smell, or even smell or taste unpleasantly to some people. The texture may not be the same as in conventional foods, particularly with unicellular organisms. These draw-backs can be overcome by adding a proportion of SCP to manufactured foods. However, even when SCP is incorporated into manufactured foods it may not have suitable characteristics such as stability, ability to bind water or fats, or ability to form gels, emulsions or foams. SCP for feed does not have to meet such strict requirements. [Pg.64]

The human tongue is sensitive to five flavors salty, sweet, bitter, sour, and umami, the taste of MSG. [Pg.72]

There are, however, a number of criticisms of these theories. Beidler argued that, as Renqvist had assumed that the magnitude of response is proportional to the amount of stimulant adsorbed per unit time, it is evident that, at equilibrium, the net velocity of adsorption is zero. It would follow that taste intensity should be zero, and the receptors completely adapted. However, Beidler showed that the receptors do not adapt completely, but reach a steady level of response that is consistent for the duration of stimulation. Therefore, he concluded that human taste-adaption is dominated by events in the central nervous system, and not by the peripheral receptor. The same facts also prove Lasarefl s assumption to be incorrect, as his experimental data also depended on a change in adaption that is not seen at the receptor level. [Pg.210]

The importance of lipophilicity to bitterness has been well established, both directly and indirectly. The importance of partitioning effects in bitterness perception has been stressed by Rubin and coworkers, and Gardner demonstrated that the threshold concentration of bitter amino acids and peptides correlates very well with molecular connectivity (which is generally regarded as a steric parameter, but is correlated with the octanol-water partition coefficient ). Studies on the surface pressure in monolayers of lipids from bovine, circumvallate papillae also indicated that there is a very good correlation between the concentration of a bitter compound that is necessary in order to give an increase in the surface pressure with the taste threshold in humans. These results and the observations of others suggested that the ability of bitter compounds to penetrate cell membranes is an important factor in bitterness perception. [Pg.318]

Studies on the biochemistry of the taste system should take into account results obtained at other levels, such as electrophysiological recordings and, particularly, behavioral responses to taste stimuli. The term sweetness should strictly be used only in studies conducted on humans, because the description of taste modality is a verbal response. It is usually concluded that positive behavioral responses in animals, that is, preferences, or electrophysiological response to a stimulus compound that is known to be sweet to man, are due to the sweet taste. This may not necessarily be true in some cases, because behavioral or electrophysiological response may result from other taste modalities. It is, therefore, critical that comparative aspects be carefully interpreted. [Pg.325]

The aim of our project was to study phloem as a source of fiber and polyphenols, and to develop a method to improve its taste without losing the potentially bioactive polyphenols. In addition, we wanted to investigate the bioavailability, cholesterolemic and antioxidative effects and safety of phloem and its phytonutrients in humans in a randomised double-blind trial. [Pg.280]

P. Overbosch and S. de Jong, A theoretical model for perceived intensity in human taste and smell. Physiol. Behav., 45 (1989) 607-613. [Pg.446]


See other pages where Taste human is mentioned: [Pg.1]    [Pg.1]    [Pg.10]    [Pg.303]    [Pg.385]    [Pg.274]    [Pg.185]    [Pg.420]    [Pg.193]    [Pg.91]    [Pg.28]    [Pg.458]    [Pg.125]    [Pg.237]    [Pg.49]    [Pg.475]    [Pg.352]    [Pg.351]    [Pg.914]    [Pg.174]    [Pg.10]    [Pg.88]    [Pg.237]    [Pg.248]    [Pg.324]    [Pg.328]    [Pg.337]    [Pg.154]    [Pg.51]    [Pg.348]   
See also in sourсe #XX -- [ Pg.15 ]




SEARCH



Human sweet taste receptors

Human taste sensations

Taste in humans

© 2024 chempedia.info