Big Chemical Encyclopedia

Chemical substances, components, reactions, process design ...

Articles Figures Tables About

Preference reversal

A most important element in computer technology is data storage. Progress in microelectronics, therefore, is directly linked to progress in data storage, that is the abihty to store large amounts of information in the smallest possible space, irreversibly or preferably reversibly. [Pg.138]

Penn, D. and Potts, W.K. (1998a) MHC-disassortative mating preferences reversed by cross-fostering. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265, 1299-1306. [Pg.140]

When drink now, abstain later and abstain now, drink later alternatives are included, we can sum up. In the morning of each day the preference ordering of sequences is never drink > drink now, abstain later > abstain now, drink later > always drink, but at six o clock my preferences reverse to drink now, abstain later > never drink > always drink > abstain now, drink later. The situation is that 1 have to choose whether to drink or not at six o clock. What do I choose If I, at six o clock, see the choice as a choice between single acts, I will drink the wine. If I see the choice as a choice of a sequence, the best sequence involves drinking today and then abstention. If I believe that what I do today has no consequences for my choice tomorrow, I will choose to drink. [Pg.43]

Another important problem is that preference reversal often seems to come quite suddenly, by some cue-dependent signaling followed by craving reactions. It is not that the possibility of consumption is necessarily immediately available some cues or signals seem to have profound influence on the preference situation. [Pg.138]

Although the specific patterns of various drugs may not fit Ainslie s model in a natural way, it performs well in explaining the general phenomenon of preference reversal. It is not clear, however, that it offers a full account of this phenomenon. Consider an example told by a former addict and cited by Lewis Yablonski (1%5). The subject s wife was about to have their first child, and he was to be present during delivery. As his... [Pg.138]

Ainslie, George, and Richard J. Hermstein. 1981. "Preference Reversal and Delayed Reinforcement." Animal Learning and Behavior 9 476-82. [Pg.205]

Kirby, Kris, and Richard J. Hermstein. 1995. "Preference Reversals Due to Myopic Discounting of Delayed Reward." Psychological Science 6 83-89. [Pg.205]

Consider next some issues of motivation. An individual s preference can be inconsistent in various ways that do not imply any kind of split self. It is possible to make a person prefer A over and C over D, even if A is essentially the same option as D, and the same as C. For instance, Mr H. mows his own lawn. His neighbor s son would mow it for 8. He wouldn t mow his neighbor s same-sized lawn for 20 (Thaler 1980). The proposed explanation for this phenomenon is that people value out-of-pocket expenses differently from opportunity cost, thus creating a normatively unjustifiable presumption in favour of the status quo. Although this particular example may yield to another explanation (Section IX), many other cases certainly fit this distinction. Thus, credit card customers may be less deterred by a cash discount to non-users than by a surcharge to users, even if the two are substantively the same (Thaler 1980). If in such cases it is possible to induce preference reversal, it is not because two parts of the person have different preferences. Rather it is because the person reacts to the way in which the options are presented, and not simply to their substantive content. [Pg.5]

Ainslie, G. and Herrnstein, R. J. (1981) Preference reversal and delayed reinforcement , Animal Learning Behavior 9,476-82. [Pg.171]

Emphasis Aims principally to eliminate influence diffusion, observe intcrfacial happenings. Prefers Tafcl region Aims principally to reduce influence interfacial happenings, observe diffusion monitor entities in solution. Prefers reversible region... [Pg.704]

Here the present counts for more relative to the near future than does the near future relative to the distant future. Because of this, preference reversal may occur. At time /. B ceases to be the most preferred option and A begins to look more attractive, right up to the time of choice. This is weakness of will the inability to do what, all things considered, one believes one should do. [Pg.55]

In deacylation, as the enzyme cleaved the phenylacyl group, phenylacetic acid was formed, which lowered the pH of the reaction medium. Base was added to maintain the starting pH. (Note Use of ammonium hydroxide led to the formation of desilylated byproducts desilylation was eliminated when bicarbonates were used.) This approach was not required in the acylation reaction. At pH above 7.5 the (R)-and (S)-amines are practically insoluble in water. Organic solvents were used to extract the free amines from the aqueous reaction medium at pH 8.0. p-Fluoro-benzoyl, 1-naphthoyl, and phenylacetyl derivatives of the racemic amine were prepared and their behavior on the chiral HPLC column was studied. Based on ease of preparation and HPLC analysis, the 1-naphthoyl derivatives (Fig. 7) were preferred. Reversed phase HPLC analysis on a Vydac-C18 analytical column used a gradient of acetonitrile (0.1% triethylamine) in water (0.05% phosphoric acid) to quantify the total amide in the reaction mixture. Chiral HPLC analysis on (S,S) Whelk-O Chiral column used isopropanol hexane (30 70) as a solvent system to separate and quantify the (R)- and (S)-enantiomers. [Pg.441]

Che, Y., and Marshall, G. R. (2006) Engineering cyclic tetrapeptides containing chimeric amino acids as preferred reverse-turn scaffolds. J. Med. Chem. 49, 111-124. [Pg.154]

In order to characterize the gold reference electrode and to estimate the potential distribution in the single-pellet cell, an electrochemical cell identical to that shown in Figure 1, but having all three electrodes made of gold, was used. A good reference electrode for the purposes of electrochemical promotion experiments must be catalytically inert in all experimental conditions, and it must have an invariant potential defined by a (preferably reversible) redox couple. Nevertheless, a... [Pg.212]

The formation of passive oxide films on the (111) surfaces of Cu and Ni has also been studied in detail by SXS [94, 95]. Measurements of Cu(lll) in 0.1 M NaCl04 (at pH 4.5) showed that the oxide exhibited a crystalline cuprite structure (CU2O) that was epitaxially aligned with the underlying Cu substrate [94]. Although a similar oxide structure was observed for oxidation in air, there were some key differences in the structure of the aqueous oxide. In particular it was found that a preferred reversed orientation of the oxide film was formed, and this indicated that oxide growth occurs at the interface between the oxide and the Cu(lll) surface... [Pg.38]

As noted by Savage (1954), the above set of gambles can be reframed in a way that shows that these preferences violate the sure-thing principle. After doing so. Savage found that his initial tendency towards choosing Al over SI and A2 over S2 disappeared. As noted by Stevenson et al. (1993), this example is one of the first cited cases of a preference reversal caused by reframing a decision, the topic discussed below. [Pg.2202]


See other pages where Preference reversal is mentioned: [Pg.127]    [Pg.135]    [Pg.136]    [Pg.136]    [Pg.137]    [Pg.139]    [Pg.139]    [Pg.140]    [Pg.144]    [Pg.144]    [Pg.155]    [Pg.331]    [Pg.5]    [Pg.163]    [Pg.572]    [Pg.97]    [Pg.97]    [Pg.834]    [Pg.4]    [Pg.76]    [Pg.2172]    [Pg.2202]    [Pg.2202]    [Pg.2202]    [Pg.2204]    [Pg.2212]    [Pg.145]    [Pg.146]    [Pg.87]   
See also in sourсe #XX -- [ Pg.145 , Pg.146 ]




SEARCH



© 2024 chempedia.info